The 2022 Winter Olympics will likely be remembered as much for their controversies as for the athletic performances. There were disputes about the fairness of the judges, the use of performance-enhancing drugs, and the appropriate age at which athletes should be allowed to enter such a physical and emotional trial. The Olympics are supposed to bring the world together, but in times of global tensions, they often prove to be just a reflection of nationalistic undercurrents. These Olympics may also have revealed something else: how cultures shape our emotions—particularly those surrounding notions of fairness.
When you first heard or read about an athlete doping or a judge bumping up the scores of his countrymen, you likely had an immediate gut reaction. What seems fair feels instinctual—we don’t even have to think about it. For that reason, it has long been assumed by psychology researchers that our fairness intuition was universal, something shared by all humans. However, recent cross-cultural studies have revealed that there are vast differences across the globe. Culture, it turns out, shapes our beliefs and actions in surprising ways.
The potential implications of the unexpected results were quickly apparent to Henrich. He knew that a vast amount of scholarly literature in the social sciences—particularly in economics and psychology—relied on the ultimatum game and similar experiments. At the heart of most of that research was the implicit assumption that the results revealed evolved psychological traits common to all humans, never mind that the test subjects were nearly always from the industrialized West. Henrich realized that if the Machiguenga results stood up, and if similar differences could be measured across other populations, this assumption of universality would have to be challenged.
Henrich had thought he would be adding a small branch to an established tree of knowledge. It turned out he was sawing at the very trunk. He began to wonder: What other certainties about “human nature” in social science research would need to be reconsidered when tested across diverse populations?
Henrich soon landed a grant from the MacArthur Foundation to take his fairness games on the road. With the help of a dozen other colleagues he led a study of 14 other small-scale societies, in locales from Tanzania to Indonesia. Differences abounded in the behavior of both players in the ultimatum game. In no society did he find people who were purely selfish (that is, who always offered the lowest amount, and never refused a split), but average offers from place to place varied widely. In some societies—ones where gift-giving is heavily used to curry favor or gain allegiance—the first player would often make seemingly overly generous offers in excess of 60 percent, and yet the second player would often reject them, behaviors almost never observed among Americans.
The research established Henrich as an up-and-coming scholar. In 2004, he was given the US Presidential Early Career Award for young scientists at the White House. But his work also made him a controversial figure. When he presented his research to the anthropology department at the University of British Columbia during a job interview a year later, he recalls a hostile reception. Anthropology is the social science most interested in cultural differences, but the young scholar’s methods of using games and statistics to test and compare cultures with the West seemed heavy-handed and invasive to some.
So instead of toeing the line, he switched teams. A few well-placed people outside the anthropology department at the University of British Columbia saw great promise in Henrich’s work and created a position for him, split between the economics department and the psychology department. It was in the psychology department that he found two kindred spirits in Steven Heine and Ara Norenzayan. Together the three set about writing a paper that they hoped would fundamentally challenge the way social scientists thought about human behavior, cognition and culture.
The universality of sports makes it an interesting case study of the effects of culture on the mind and on group behavior. It is not surprising that when athletes gather from around the world for an event like the Olympics, we may witness behavior that seems alien to us. The rules for a sport may be the same around the world, but the behavior of athletes will still be influenced by differing notions of fairness. Those from cultures where money is a proxy for status, for instance, may bend the rules for financial gain. Other athletes from countries with high levels of nationalism may tolerate bending the rules because they are motivated to win for their in-group at all costs. We won’t fully understand the behavior of athletes and teams until we understand how culture shapes their (and our) behavior.